Social proximity for new friendships
I have asked the SIMSOC list for help on how to calculate social proximity with the purpose of growing a friendship network in an agent-based simulation model, specifying a set of elements that characterise my agents. Social proximity as I plan to use it is studied in different bodies of literature under different names: homophily, conformity, and social distance (well, the inverse thereof). The frame here is, in (WJ) words:
there is an increased interest for integrating behaviour theoretical insights in a modelling framework. Specifically when processes of behavioural change are the topic of interest, be it farmers’ choice behaviour, recycling behaviour or citizens’ future mobility, the aforementioned examples indicate that the integration of different psychological mechanisms in a modelling framework contributes to the development of policy relevant models. Because the psychological drivers and dynamics of behaviour are included in these models, they can address social psychological responses to policy that go beyond the capacity of traditional economic and spatial models. (WJ)
My attempt was motivated by practical reasons, being in the process of implementing a simulation where this will be used as an ingredient. In other words, I need to calculate it to let a network where another phenomenon is being studied. To make it clear, what I needed was a set of weights that could, independent of context (and I know that sounds wrong; there might be nothing about this subject that is independent from context), weight the factors against each other. For example, I could use a statement telling that the difference in age, measured in years, is to be weighted twice as much as a difference in gender.
My starting point was the review paper from Dunbar (2018) which proposed ethnicity, age, religion, education, and social values as the basis for friendship - but, even as it ranked the factors, didn’t extend to proposing numerical weights for those.
In my simulation, the variables that my agents have are gender, education-level (4 levels), wealth-level (4 levels), job-level (4 levels), age, number-of-children
. I also have friendship networks which can be used for several kinds of calculations; but as the network would also extend on the basis of social proximity, I am afraid of logical recursion there. Note that I am purposefully detaching this discussion from the main purpose of the simulation to keep the matter self-contained.
So, the objective was to find function of difference in gender, education-level, wealth-level, job-level, age, number-of-children, number-of-friends
returning a measure between 0 and 1. Something practical, a snippet of code with factor weights; in short, something that would take the place of my 1.0
s in the following netlogo snippet:
to-report factors-social-proximity
report (list
; var-name weight normalized-reporter
(list "age" 1.0 [ -> ifelse-value (abs (age - [ age ] of myself) > 18) [ 0 ] [ 1 - abs (age - [ age ] of myself) / 18 ] ])
(list "gender" 1.0 [ -> ifelse-value (male? = [ male? ] of myself) [ 1 ][ 0 ] ])
(list "wealth" 1.0 [ -> ifelse-value (wealth-level = [ wealth-level ] of myself) [ 1 ][ 0 ] ])
(list "job" 1.0 [ -> ifelse-value (job-level = [ job-level ] of myself) [ 1 ][ 0 ] ])
(list "education" 1.0 [ -> ifelse-value (education-level = [ education-level ] of myself) [ 1 ][ 0 ] ])
Here, I sum up the answers from the SIMSOC list and from direct connections, together with a few references from the literature. In short, what I find is:
1 no precedent has emerged in the sense of an algorithm; this comes as a surprise and I remain confident that something might be found if we keep looking.
2 there’s no “one fit all” solution; at the very least, gender difference should make the calculation diverge on different algorithms
Maybe the single most important point that was made, on which I completely agree, is the following:
What you might want to do is try multiple schemes and see how sensitive your model is to this. (KC)
Factors suggested
Collaboration is a basis for community and it generally requires both social proximity and spatial proximity. However, there are some scenarios where social proximity seems more important and others where spatial proximity seems more important. (It all gets very confusing when so much communication is done between people using virtual means!) (…)
…so people are more likely to socialise with each other and have social proximity if they go to the same places and or have the same interests. (AT)
Social proximity, as I am looking for it, seems to be the inverse of social distance - this is measured since the 1930’s with the Bogardus scale. The Bogardus scale aims to measure social distance towards abstract groups characterized by, for example, an ethnic or religious connotation. It works through agreement or disagreement with a series of statements about potential joint activities with members of those group in roles ranging from spouse to exclusion. I haven’t found, however, any direct correlation or causation of factors leading to a value in the Bogardus scale. Moreover, social distance is designed towards groups, and thus not immediately leading to the formation of new friendship.
there is no standard measure. Also for friendship networks - people often use not just number of friends in common - but also number of friends not in common - people also consider triad as much of the work on new tie formation is based on triad closure.
how much gender, age, ethnicity, religion etc are weighted varies by culture. Also withing cultures these factors are non-linearly related. For example, gender based friendship is low before 7 years of age, then very strong then it drops again in the 30’s in some cultures. In other cultures religious based ties are high until one is 18 and then high again after age 60. (KC)
So, summing up, here are the factors suggested:
- spatial proximity
- education
- also number of friends not in common, aka triad closure (KC)
Of these factors, our model cannot consider spatial proximity - this re-enters the algorithm anyway through the importance of co-workers (and schoolmates). Triad closure seems a good addition to what we had initially considered. The importance of existing relationships is also indirectly supported by Chen et al. (2009):
relationship based algorithms outperform content similarity ones in terms of user response.
Interaction of factors
…the probability of two agents interacting (is) dependent on their similarity. This produces a network where in principle all agents can interact, but the more similar the agent, the more likely they interact. (WJ)
Ok for similarity, but the probability might depend on the specific kind of interaction. Children don’t look for similar tutors, most people look for complementarity rather than for similarity. Palchykov et al. (2012) notice peaks in selection of “best friends” of the opposite gender - although this decreases with the second, third friend and so on. All this is a characteristic of the link more than the network, and the same individual could very well express different attitudes on different links. Akerlof (1997), while calculating an utility function for a representative agent in social interaction, considers separately both conformism and status seeking as giving rise to opposite utility functions for social interaction. However, Akerlof doesn’t have the issue of weights - he coalesces it all in a single value of social position (whose natural interpretation is educational attainment).
With these kind of variables I would be looking for a socially plausible set of metrics. So for example, ‘birds of a feather flock together’ would lead to stronger bonds between people with similar characteristics. Conversely, ‘opposites attract’ would allow for example for the prevalence of inter gender relationships in the family as well as for organic solidarities in the form of complementary divisions of labour. (AP)
social drivers such as conformism, anti-conformism and non-conformism as well as social learning strategies, imitation and social comparison processes.
Individuals usually participate in multiple networks of each type, and one of the key differentiators might be the number of different such networks, and the similarity of these networks between individuals. (AP)
Age and gender have a complex and culture-dependent relation.
how much gender, age, ethnicity, religion etc are weighted varies by culture. Also within cultures these factors are non-linearly related. For example, gender based frienship is low before 7 years of age, then very strong then it drops again in the 30’s in some cultures. In other cultures religious based ties are high until one is 18 and then high again after age 60. (KC)
In other words, focusing on social distance tells just half of the story. While several social interactions seem to be dictated by distance
personally am a bit suspicious of ‘social distance’. It takes us part of the way, but not the whole way. It uses only ascribed characteristics. What about complementarity, what about admiration? (HG)
Relations with a different age class and gender, intuitively, might enjoy complementarity more than similarity - however, we need some data to support and quantify this effect.
Effects suggested:
- ‘birds of a feather flock together’
- ‘opposites attract’
Experimental approach
Perhaps we could resort to an experimental approach - given time and resources. On this line, we could seek help from projects as IBSEN (https://ibsen-h2020.eu/).
When a simulation needs to be fed real data, often an experiment is a good approach. To measure feeling of closeness, there could be different approaches.
We could give respondents photos of all combinations of variables under investigation (OK, there are about 2,000 combinations, let´s say they judge 100 /randomly/ selected) with short CV accenting characteristics under investigation and measure feeling of closeness for pairs of respondent´s and object´s characteristics. You might measure feeling on scale 0-100 or via Q-sort technique (every respondent sort 100 photos according pseudo normal distribution: 5 closest, 5 farthest, 7 less close, 7 less far, 9 even less close, 9 even less far … etc.) (FK)
The result: our code for social proximity
modelling human behaviour in computer models forces the researcher to develop running code, and hence make inferences on the causal mechanisms for the simulated agent (WJ)
… thus, after the discussion, I have modified the initial snippet as follows. Age and genders stay, for the moment, the same, as this model is about friendship and not (not only, not necessarily) about “best friends.” I have kept wealth as one of the factor, but I have left out, assigning them a weight of zero, job, education, and retired state, as they are strongly correlated in the model. The main addition here is the snippet testing for potential triad closure.
to-report factors-social-proximity ; person reporter. Self is ego, myself is alter
let ego self
let alter myself
report (list
; var-name weight normalized-reporter
(list "age" 1.0 [ -> ifelse-value (abs (age - [ age ] of ego) > 18) [ 0 ] [ 1 - abs (age - [ age ] of ego) / 18 ] ])
(list "gender" 1.0 [ -> ifelse-value (male? = [ male? ] of ego) [ 1 ][ 0 ] ])
(list "wealth" 1.0 [ -> ifelse-value (wealth-level = [ wealth-level ] of ego) [ 1 ][ 0 ] ])
(list "job" 0.0 [ -> ifelse-value (job-level = [ job-level ] of ego) [ 1 ][ 0 ] ])
(list "education" 1.0 [ -> ifelse-value (education-level = [ education-level ] of ego) [ 1 ][ 0 ] ])
(list "retired" 0.0 [ -> ifelse-value (retired? = [ retired? ] of ego) [ 1 ][ 0 ] ])
(list "closure" 1.0 [ -> ifelse-value (any? (other [ friendship-link-neighbors ] of alter) with
[ friendship-link-neighbor? ego ]) [ 1 ][ 0 ] ])
)
end
Acknowledgements
Thanks to all who answered; snippets copied from the message I recevied are marked with the initial of the author: Alan Penn (AP), Andy Turner (AT), Frantisek Kalvas (FK), Gertjan Hofstede (HG), Kathleen Carley (KC), Wander Jager (WJ). They are used, of course, in my interpretation; most of the messages can be retrieved from the SIMSOC list. I am also indebted to Ignacio Tamarit Ramírez.
Bibliography
Akerlof, G. A. (1997). Social Distance and Social Decisions. Econometrica, 65(5), 1005–1027. https://doi.org/10.2307/2171877
Chen, J., Geyer, W., Dugan, C., Muller, M., & Guy, I. (2009). Make new friends, but keep the old: recommending people on social networking sites (pp. 201–210). Presented at the Proceedings of the 27th international conference on Human factors in computing systems, ACM. https://doi.org/10.1145/1518701.1518735
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2018). The Anatomy of Friendship. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(1), 32–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.10.004
Jager, W. (2017). Enhancing the Realism of Simulation (EROS): On Implementing and Developing Psychological Theory in Social Simulation. Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, 20(3). https://doi.org/10.18564/jasss.3522
Palchykov, V., Kaski, K., Kertész, J., Barabási, A.-L., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2012). Sex differences in intimate relationships. Scientific Reports, 2, 370. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep00370
Sherif, M., & Hovland, C. I. (1961). Social judgment: Assimilation and contrast effects in communication and attitude change. Oxford, England: Yale Univer. Press.
Social proximity for new friendships
I have asked the SIMSOC list for help on how to calculate social proximity with the purpose of growing a friendship network in an agent-based simulation model, specifying a set of elements that characterise my agents. Social proximity as I plan to use it is studied in different bodies of literature under different names: homophily, conformity, and social distance (well, the inverse thereof). The frame here is, in (WJ) words:
My attempt was motivated by practical reasons, being in the process of implementing a simulation where this will be used as an ingredient. In other words, I need to calculate it to let a network where another phenomenon is being studied. To make it clear, what I needed was a set of weights that could, independent of context (and I know that sounds wrong; there might be nothing about this subject that is independent from context), weight the factors against each other. For example, I could use a statement telling that the difference in age, measured in years, is to be weighted twice as much as a difference in gender.
My starting point was the review paper from Dunbar (2018) which proposed ethnicity, age, religion, education, and social values as the basis for friendship - but, even as it ranked the factors, didn’t extend to proposing numerical weights for those.
In my simulation, the variables that my agents have are
gender, education-level (4 levels), wealth-level (4 levels), job-level (4 levels), age, number-of-children
. I also have friendship networks which can be used for several kinds of calculations; but as the network would also extend on the basis of social proximity, I am afraid of logical recursion there. Note that I am purposefully detaching this discussion from the main purpose of the simulation to keep the matter self-contained.So, the objective was to find function of difference in
gender, education-level, wealth-level, job-level, age, number-of-children, number-of-friends
returning a measure between 0 and 1. Something practical, a snippet of code with factor weights; in short, something that would take the place of my1.0
s in the following netlogo snippet:Here, I sum up the answers from the SIMSOC list and from direct connections, together with a few references from the literature. In short, what I find is:
1 no precedent has emerged in the sense of an algorithm; this comes as a surprise and I remain confident that something might be found if we keep looking.
2 there’s no “one fit all” solution; at the very least, gender difference should make the calculation diverge on different algorithms
Maybe the single most important point that was made, on which I completely agree, is the following:
Factors suggested
Social proximity, as I am looking for it, seems to be the inverse of social distance - this is measured since the 1930’s with the Bogardus scale. The Bogardus scale aims to measure social distance towards abstract groups characterized by, for example, an ethnic or religious connotation. It works through agreement or disagreement with a series of statements about potential joint activities with members of those group in roles ranging from spouse to exclusion. I haven’t found, however, any direct correlation or causation of factors leading to a value in the Bogardus scale. Moreover, social distance is designed towards groups, and thus not immediately leading to the formation of new friendship.
So, summing up, here are the factors suggested:
Of these factors, our model cannot consider spatial proximity - this re-enters the algorithm anyway through the importance of co-workers (and schoolmates). Triad closure seems a good addition to what we had initially considered. The importance of existing relationships is also indirectly supported by Chen et al. (2009):
Interaction of factors
Ok for similarity, but the probability might depend on the specific kind of interaction. Children don’t look for similar tutors, most people look for complementarity rather than for similarity. Palchykov et al. (2012) notice peaks in selection of “best friends” of the opposite gender - although this decreases with the second, third friend and so on. All this is a characteristic of the link more than the network, and the same individual could very well express different attitudes on different links. Akerlof (1997), while calculating an utility function for a representative agent in social interaction, considers separately both conformism and status seeking as giving rise to opposite utility functions for social interaction. However, Akerlof doesn’t have the issue of weights - he coalesces it all in a single value of social position (whose natural interpretation is educational attainment).
Age and gender have a complex and culture-dependent relation.
In other words, focusing on social distance tells just half of the story. While several social interactions seem to be dictated by distance
Relations with a different age class and gender, intuitively, might enjoy complementarity more than similarity - however, we need some data to support and quantify this effect.
Effects suggested:
Experimental approach
Perhaps we could resort to an experimental approach - given time and resources. On this line, we could seek help from projects as IBSEN (https://ibsen-h2020.eu/).
The result: our code for social proximity
… thus, after the discussion, I have modified the initial snippet as follows. Age and genders stay, for the moment, the same, as this model is about friendship and not (not only, not necessarily) about “best friends.” I have kept wealth as one of the factor, but I have left out, assigning them a weight of zero, job, education, and retired state, as they are strongly correlated in the model. The main addition here is the snippet testing for potential triad closure.
Acknowledgements
Thanks to all who answered; snippets copied from the message I recevied are marked with the initial of the author: Alan Penn (AP), Andy Turner (AT), Frantisek Kalvas (FK), Gertjan Hofstede (HG), Kathleen Carley (KC), Wander Jager (WJ). They are used, of course, in my interpretation; most of the messages can be retrieved from the SIMSOC list. I am also indebted to Ignacio Tamarit Ramírez.
Bibliography
Akerlof, G. A. (1997). Social Distance and Social Decisions. Econometrica, 65(5), 1005–1027. https://doi.org/10.2307/2171877
Chen, J., Geyer, W., Dugan, C., Muller, M., & Guy, I. (2009). Make new friends, but keep the old: recommending people on social networking sites (pp. 201–210). Presented at the Proceedings of the 27th international conference on Human factors in computing systems, ACM. https://doi.org/10.1145/1518701.1518735
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2018). The Anatomy of Friendship. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(1), 32–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.10.004
Jager, W. (2017). Enhancing the Realism of Simulation (EROS): On Implementing and Developing Psychological Theory in Social Simulation. Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, 20(3). https://doi.org/10.18564/jasss.3522
Palchykov, V., Kaski, K., Kertész, J., Barabási, A.-L., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2012). Sex differences in intimate relationships. Scientific Reports, 2, 370. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep00370
Sherif, M., & Hovland, C. I. (1961). Social judgment: Assimilation and contrast effects in communication and attitude change. Oxford, England: Yale Univer. Press.